Matthew Cobb is the author of the new book The Idea of the Brain: The Past and Future of Neuroscience. His other books include Life's Greatest Secret and Eleven Days in August. He is a professor in the School of Biological Sciences at the University of Manchester. He lives in England.
Q: You write that "when it comes to really
understanding what happens in a brain at the level of neuronal networks and
their component cells, or to being able to predict what will happen when the
activity of a particular network is altered, we are still at the very
beginning." Why is that?
A: I suppose it’s because it’s far more complicated
than we ever imagined!
We have some high-level understanding of the broad
function of some regions, and in experimental animals, such as mice, we can
measure and alter the activity of individual cells. This has enabled scientists
to change a pleasant memory into an unpleasant one, or even to erase a memory
completely.
But impressive as such achievements are, they are not
yet properly integrated into a detailed, cell-level vision – a theory, if you
like – of what is happening.
The work of Eve Marder at Brandeis University highlights
the problem. For decades, she has been studying the lobster’s stomach, which
contains around 30 neurons that produce rhythmic patterns of activity.
Despite a vast amount of work, she and her colleagues
are unable to explain what is the link between the structure of these networks
and the rhythms they produce, nor to predict on the basis of computer models
how changing the activity of one part of the network will alter its activity.
If we don’t understand the lobster’s stomach, it’s
hard to see how we can progress in understanding more complex brains.
Q: You divide the book in three sections, Past,
Present (starting in 1950), and Future. How did you decide on the book's
structure?
A: The military have a saying – “No plan survives
first engagement with the enemy.” Books can be a bit like that. You start off
with a plan – in my case it was a relatively straightforward chronological
account of progress in neuroscience – and then you realize that maybe another
approach is needed.
In this case, the question of the machine metaphors
used by scientists down the ages to understand the brain was not at the heart
of the initial plan. But it emerged as I tried to struggle with the material,
and wanted to find an underlying theme that would hold everything together.
Once that became the thread running through the book, I
then realized I could lump everything since 1950 as “the present,” because we
have been working with the computational metaphor – seeing the brain as
some kind of computer – since that date.
That also led me to write the six chapters in the
Present section as parallel, thematic chapters, rather than chronologically.
The idea of writing about “the future” was a bit more
audacious, and what I argue there is very much a personal view. As I say in the
Introduction, I expect many colleagues will disagree with this section, and think
it is provocative.
But that was my intention, to provoke thinking about
where we are going, and what kind of new metaphors we need to understand how
the brain works, or even if everything is ticketty-boo and we don’t need to
worry much, because everything will turn out fine.
With around 100,000 neuroscientists around the world,
I imagine there are least that many ideas about the future!
Q: How did you research this book, and did you learn
anything that particularly surprised or intrigued you?
A: I was particularly intrigued by the personality of
Alfred Smee, a very odd 19th century inventor who worked for the Bank of
England and features in the chapter on Electricity.
Smee – who I had bit heard of before working on the
book – came up with various models for how the brain might work, which were
both striking and obscure. He produced brass devices which, he claimed, could
represent an idea and which, if they were all put together would be able to
imitate thought processes.
But when you look at his diagrams, it is hard to see
how they might work. He had some insights, but they also look rather loopy from
the present day.
The other thing that surprised me was the way that
Francis Crick kept on barging into the chapters in the “Present” section – in
fact he plays a key role in every chapter, except the one on neurotransmitters.
I knew that Crick had turned to neuroscience in the
1970s, but I had not realized quite how influential he had been, in particular
in the development of computer programs that can parallel how nervous systems
process information, and in the development of connectomes – highly detailed
maps of the brain.
My previous book, Life’s Greatest Secret, on the
cracking of the genetic code, turned into a paeon of praise to Crick (that was
not my intention), and I was surprised to discover that the same thing happened
to the second half of The Idea of the Brain, too.
Q: What do you see looking ahead when it comes to
research about the brain?
A: As I’ve indicated, this is the subject of the brief
final part of the book.
At one end, I expect there to be great excitement as
we acquire ever more precise control of measuring and activating cells in
brains, including in quite complex mammalian brains.
I would predict that Doris Tsao, who works on face
detection in monkeys (and humans) will continue to provide some extraordinary
insights into how this fundamental process operates, and what exactly the brain
detects when it sees a face.
At the other end of the scale, work on simple brains –
in maggots, worms, and flies – will eventually provide us with such detailed
information about structure and function that the complex models currently
being developed to try and explain all sorts of things about the human brain
will be able to be tested on these simple systems.
At the moment, some of the theoreticians operate in a
vacuum because we don’t have sufficient understanding to be able to put their
models to the test.
What I am confident we will not see – at least in my
lifetime – is an understanding of how consciousness emerges from the human
brain, nor do I expect to see conscious machines. Both those possibilities will
not be realized for centuries, I predict.
Q: What are you working on now?
A: I am about to start publicizing another book, which
is out at the end of May, called Smell: A Very Short Introduction. It is one of
those little popular books published by Oxford University Press, and it covers
my day job, which is understanding the sense of smell in humans and animals.
It was particularly interesting to write, because one
chapter deals with the role of smell in culture and history, which is not
something I have previously explored.
In June, there may be a bit of excitement over the publication
of a book I translated and collaborated upon, Michel Morange’s The Black Box of Biology: A History of the Molecular Revolution, published by Harvard University
Press.
This is a history of molecular biology and contains up
to the minute accounts of how biology has been transformed over the past
decades.
Having three books published in a matter of months is
not recommended – it makes for a very chaotic diary, even in our current times!
In terms of what I am writing, it’s a change of gear from
The Idea of the Brain – my new book is a social history of genetic engineering
and the various fears that have been conjured up by it, from recombinant DNA in
the 1970s to CRISPR and gene drives today.
The book – currently called Life, Edited – looks both
at the science and the politics of this amazing technology which has
transformed biology, is changing medicine and could change the whole planet.
I’m just at the beginning of this and have enjoyed
immersing myself in alternative ideas from the 1970s – Science for the People!
Right on!
Q: Anything else we should know?
A: Stay safe! Wash your hands, keep your distance, try
to keep happy and well.
And buy books – not just mine (of course), but all
sorts of books. So many great books have disappeared in the current maelstrom,
but they deserve our support.
With bookstores shut, it is hard to know what books
are out – check out newspaper websites, or your favorite authors on Twitter.
And don’t forget your local bookstore – they may still do rapid deliveries, at
a time when A Certain Website is downgrading its book deliveries.
--Interview with Deborah Kalb. Here's a previous Q&A with Matthew Cobb.
No comments:
Post a Comment